Infants, in their nascent state of cognitive development, are yet to grasp the concept that the reflection cast upon the mirror is none other than their own likeness. The aptitude to discern oneself in the mirror commonly flourishes between the tender age of 18 to 24 months.
Take a closer look now
The enigma of infants’ failure to acknowledge their own reflections in the looking glass presents a captivating phenomenon that unfurls profound glimpses into their cognitive maturation. Though the initial response barely grazed the surface of this notion, let us plunge further into the underlying rationales that underpin this captivating conduct.
In the early stages of life, infants are devoid of self-awareness and fail to comprehend the notion of a reflection portraying their own essence. The capacity to acknowledge one’s own reflection in the mirror is commonly referred to as self-recognition, a cognitive achievement that demands a gradual maturation process.
In the realm of cognitive development, as expounded by the esteemed scholar Jean Piaget, the tender years of infancy witness a progression through diverse stages. The particular stage germane to the phenomenon of mirror self-recognition is none other than the sensorimotor stage, which spans from the moment of birth until approximately two years of age. Throughout this span, infants undergo a gradual maturation in the realm of object permanence, wherein they acquire the comprehension that objects endure in existence even when concealed from view. And it is precisely within this stage that the emergence of self-recognition takes its nascent form.
The evaluation known as the mirror test serves as a means to gauge one’s self-perception, frequently employed by scholars in the field. Originally proposed by the esteemed psychologist Gordon Gallup, this examination entails the application of a discreet crimson dot upon an infant’s visage, followed by keen observation of their subsequent response upon viewing their own reflection. The revelation of self-awareness is evidenced by the child’s instinctual inclination to engage with and scrutinize the marked area upon their own countenance.
- Charles Cooley’s famous quote: “I am not what I think I am, and I am not what you think I am. I am what I think you think I am.” This quote emphasizes the importance of social interaction in developing our The importance of self-awareness. Babies rely on social cues and interactions to shape their understanding of themselves, which is why mirror tests play an important role.
The emergence of self-recognition in infants, typically occurring between the ages of 18 to 24 months, is subject to a range of influences encompassing cultural customs, individual disparities, and developmental hindrances. Furthermore, the extent and regularity of exposure to mirrors may exert a considerable influence on the timing of a child’s recognition of their own reflection.
To provide a visually appealing representation, let’s consider a table highlighting some interesting facts related to mirror self-recognition:
Fact | Explanation |
---|---|
Mirror self-recognition emerges | Typically between 18 to 24 months |
Mirror test | A method to assess self-recognition by marking an infant’s face and observing their reaction |
Social interaction | Plays a pivotal role in shaping a baby’s understanding of self |
Variations in development | Cultural practices, individual differences, and delays can affect the timing of self-recognition |
Exposure to mirrors | Quality and frequency of mirror exposure can impact self-recognition development |
In conclusion, babies’ inability to recognize themselves in the mirror stems from their early stage of cognitive development and lack of self-awareness. However, as they grow and acquire new cognitive abilities, they gradually develop the capacity to understand and recognize their own reflection.
Associated video
This video shows various animals seeing themselves in a mirror and their reactions to the experience. Some animals, such as elephants and dolphins, display self-awareness and inspect their bodies, while others like dogs and lions show curiosity but do not seem to recognize themselves. Interestingly, some animals, such as cows and pigs, mistake their reflections for friends, while monkeys and squirrels react aggressively towards their own image. The video also explains how some animals, like magpies and chimpanzees, can recognize themselves in a mirror, while others, like horses and snakes, have different vision that may make the mirror test confusing.
See additional response choices
Up until about 2 years of age, babies don’t typically react to the mark, which leads scientists to determine that the baby lacks the cognitive ability to connect their reflection with their real body. So for all that face-to-face time, Baby doesn’t stare at the mirror because they are happy to see themself.
It can disrupt their sleep
Babies cannot look in the mirror because it can disrupt their sleep. This is because babies are attracted to movements and when they see their reflection in the mirror, they will play with it which can disrupt their sleep.
I’m sure you’ll be interested
Reflective self-awareness emerges between 15 and 18 months of age when children begin to match their own facial and/or body movements with the image of themselves in a mirror, exhibiting mirror self-recognition (see Loveland, 1986, Mitchell, 1993, Rochat, 1995b for alternative interpretations).